Legare, C. H., and Gelman, S. A. (2009). South African children’s understanding of AIDS and flu: Investigating conceptual understanding of cause, treatment and prevention. Journal of Cognition and Culture, 9, 333–346.
Leiserowitz, A., Maibach, E., Roser-Renouf, C., Feinberg, G. and Howe, P. (2013). Global warming’s six Americas. New Haven, CT: Yale Project on Climate Change Communication.
Lewandowsky, S., Ecker, U. K., Seifert, C. M., Schwarz, N., and Cook, J. (2012). Misinformation and its correction continued influence and successful debiasing. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 13, 106–131.
Lewandowsky, S., Gignac, G. E., and Vaughan, S. (2013). The pivotal role of perceived scientific consensus in acceptance of science. Nature Climate Change, 3, 399–404.
Lewis, E. L., and Linn, M. C. (1994). Heat energy and temperature concepts of adolescents, adults, and experts: Implications for curricular improvements. Journal of Research in Science Teaching, 31, 657–677.
Li, Y., Johnson, E. J., and Zaval, L. (2011). Local warming: Daily temperature change influences belief in global warming. Psychological Science, 22, 454–459.
Libarkin, J. C., and Anderson, S. W. (2005). Assessment of learning in entry-level geoscience courses: Results from the Geoscience Concept Inventory. Journal of Geoscience Education, 53, 394–401.
Libarkin, J. C., Anderson, S. W., Dahl, J., Beilfuss, M., Boone, W., and Kurdziel, J. P (2005). Qualitative analysis of college students’ ideas about the earth: Interviews and open-ended questionnaires. Journal of Geoscience Education, 53, 17–26.
Libarkin, J. C., and Schneps, M. H. (2012). Elementary children’s retrodictive reasoning about earth science. International Electronic Journal of Elementary Education, 5, 47–62.
Lindberg, D. C. (1976). Theories of vision from al-Kindi to Kepler. Chicago: University of Chicago Press.
Liu, X., and MacIsaac, D. (2005). An investigation of factors affecting the degree of naive impetus theory application. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 14, 101–116.
Livingston, E., and Zylke, J. W. (2012). JAMA obesity theme issue. Journal of the American Medical Association, 307, 970–971.
Lombardi, D., and Sinatra, G. M. (2012). College students’ perceptions about the plausibility of human-induced climate change. Research in Science Education, 42, 201–217.
Lusk, J. (2015). Food demand survey: January 2015. Stillwater Oklahoma State University Department of Agricultural Economics.
MacAvoy, A. (2015). Pentagon plans to exhume, identify hundreds killed in bombing of Pearl Harbor. www.huffingtonpost.com/2015/04/14/pentagon-pearl-harbor-identify_n_7066902.html.
MacDonald, T., and Wiley, E. O. (2012). Communicating phylogeny: Evolutionary tree diagrams in museums. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 5, 14–28.
MacFadden, B. J., Oviedo, L. H., Seymour, G. M., and Ellis, S. (2012). Fossil horses, orthogenesis, and communicating evolution in museums. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 5, 29–37.
Mahajan, N., Barnes, J. L., Blanco, M., and Santos, L. R. (2009). Enumeration of objects and substances in non-human primates: Experiments with brown lemurs (Eulemurfulvus). Developmental Science, 12, 920–928.
Marques, L., and Thompson, D. (1997). Misconceptions and conceptual changes concerning continental drift and plate tectonics among Portuguese students aged 16–17. Research in Science and Technological Education, 15, 195–222.
Marshak, S. (2009). Essentials of geology. New York: W. W. Norton and Company.
Marvin, U. (1973). Continental drift: The evolution of a concept. Washington, DC: Smithsonian Institution Press.
Masson, M. E., Bub, D. N., and Lalonde, C. E. (2011). Video-game training and naive reasoning about object motion. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 25, 166–173.
Masson, S., Potvin, P., Riopel, M., and Foisy, L. M. B. (2014). Differences in brain activation between novices and experts in science during a task involving a common misconception in electricity. Mind, Brain, and Education, 8, 44–55.
Masson, S., and Vazquez-Abad, J. (2006). Integrating history of science in science education through historical microworlds to promote conceptual change. Journal of Science Education and Technology, 15, 257–268.
Mayo Clinic (2014). Causes of frostbite. Retrieved from www.mayoclinic.org/diseases-conditions/frostbite/basics/causes/con-20034608.
Mayr, E. (1982). The growth of biological thought: Diversity, evolution, and inheritance. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Mayr, E. (2001). What evolution is. New York: Basic Books.
Mazens, K., and Lautrey, J. (2003). Conceptual change in physics: Children’s naive representations of sound. Cognitive Development, 18, 159–176.
McCloskey, M. (1983a). Intuitive physics. Scientific American, 248, 122–130.
McCloskey, M. (1983b). Naive theories of motion. In D. Gentner and A. L. Stevens (eds.), Mental models, 299–324. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
McCloskey, M., Caramazza, A., and Green, B. (1980). Curvilinear motion in the absence of external forces: Naive beliefs about the motion of objects. Science, 210, 1139–1141.
McFerran, B., and Mukhopadhyay, A. (2013). Lay theories of obesity predict actual body mass. Psychological Science, 24, 1428–1436.
McNeil, N. M., Fyfe, E. R., and Dunwiddie, A. E. (2015). Arithmetic practice can be modified to promote understanding of mathematical equivalence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 107, 423–436.
Mead, L. S., and Mates, A. (2009). Why science standards are important to a strong science curriculum and how states measure up. Evolution: Education and Outreach, 2, 359–371.
Medin, D., Waxman, S., Woodring, J., and Washinawatok, K. (2010). Human-centeredness is not a universal feature of young children’s reasoning: Culture and experience matter when reasoning about biological entities. Cognitive Development, 25, 197–207.
Meir, E., Perry, J, Herron, J. C., and Kingsolver, J. (2007). College students’ misconceptions about evolutionary trees. American Biology Teacher, 69, 71–76.
Mendes, N., Rakoczy, H., and Call, J. (2008). Ape metaphysics: Object individuation without language. Cognition, 106, 730–749.
Mermelstein, E., and Meyer, E. (1969). Conservation training techniques and their effects on different populations. Child Development, 40, 471–490.